Vibrissal touch in the Etruscan shrew

From Scholarpedia
Claudia Roth-Alpermann and Michael Brecht (2009), Scholarpedia, 4(11):6830. doi:10.4249/scholarpedia.6830 revision #150470 [link to/cite this article]
Jump to: navigation, search
Post-publication activity

Curator: Michael Brecht

Figure 1: The Etruscan shrew. (A) Etruscan shrew with whisker fan. (B) Frontal view of the shrew’s head. (C) High magnification view of the microvibrissae surrounding the shrew’s mouth.

The Etruscan shrew Suncus etruscus (also known as white-toothed pygmy shrew) is the smallest terrestrial mammal with a body weight of 2 g and a body length of around 4 cm without tail (Figure 1A). Shrews feed on insects and they use the sense of touch to detect and hunt prey. The elongated rostrum of the shrew has long whiskers referred to as macrovibrissae (Figure 1A, B); the shrew’s mouth is surrounded by a dense array of short whiskers, the so-called microvibrissae (Figure 1C).


Contents

Behavioral ecology

Etruscan shrews belong to the family of Soricidae (shrews) and therein to the subfamily of Crocidurinae (white-toothed shrews)(Wilson and Reeder, 2005). It is widely believed that among recent mammals, shrews represent the closest relative to the ancestor of all placental mammals and the earliest shrew-like fossils date back approximately 70 to 100 million years ago (Archibald et al., 2001)

Figure 2: Range map of the Etruscan shrew. Adapted from Aulagnier et al., 2008.

Etruscan shrews can be found from the Mediterranean to Southeast Asia in a belt extending between 10° and 30°N latitude (Figure 2). Their habitat includes scrub, open forest and grassland environments. In the Mediterranean region it prefers abandoned olive groves, vineyards, and other cultivated areas overrun by shrubs (Aulagnier et al., 2008). Being hunted by predatory birds and owls, shrews try to avoid moving uncovered in the open field, but rather seek shelter in old dry stone walls, under leaves or pieces of bark on the ground or in loose soil. They are specialized for a life in slits found in stone walls or piles of rock and they are able to enter and capture prey in slits as thin as 7 millimeters. Etruscan shrews successfully hunt and feed on insects that have almost the same body size as themselves and crickets are amongst their preferred food.

The small body size of Etruscan shrews goes along with an extraordinarily high energy turnover. They feed up to 25 times a day and consume more than their own body weight in food. Heart, respiratory system and skeletal muscles are functionally and structurally adapted to meet the enormous metabolic needs (Jürgens, 2002). In case of food restriction and at low ambient temperature, Etruscan shrews can reduce their body temperature and enter a resting state called “torpor” to cut down their energy expenditure (Fons et al., 1997). Traditionally, shrews (including Suncus etruscus) have been regarded as nocturnal animals. However, probably due to their constant food requirement, they may actually have a polyphasic circadian activity pattern with frequent activity bouts distributed over a period of 24 hours. This means that shrews have to be able to successfully hunt in twilight as well as in darkness, i.e., under conditions where vision is of limited use and, indeed, sight only seems to play a minor role. Work on relative shrew species from the Crocidura genus implicated vibrissae in navigation, but argued against the presence of echolocation in these animals (Grünwald, 1969).

A synopsis of sensory ecology of Etruscan shrews suggests the following picture: They live and hunt in slits inaccessible to larger animals. Here they rely on touch rather than on long-distance sensory modalities such as vision. Thus, shrews can be regarded as short-range/high-speed animals.

Tactile prey capture behavior

Etruscan shrew prey capture is guided by tactile cues (Anjum and Brecht, 2006). In a laboratory setting, hunting was filmed in total darkness under infrared illumination while crickets were offered as prey. These experiments demonstrated that Etruscan shrews attack in a precise and fast manner and that they need their whiskers to hunt successfully.

Spatio-temporal analysis of attacks on crickets

Figure 3: Spatial and temporal analysis of shrew attacks. Shrew attacks are selectively placed on the thorax of crickets. (A) Attack histogram showing the location of bites. (B) Bite mark positions (yellow squares superimposed on a cricket photograph) and bite mark histogram. (C) Histogram of attack latencies (time to completion of first attacks) showing the time from encounter to the end point of an attack on a cricket. (D) Histogram of inter-attack intervals (time from attack end point to the beginning point of the successive attack.

Spatial attack characteristics. Etruscan shrews place their attacks selectively on the cricket’s thorax (Figure 3 A, B) and manage to keep this precision regardless of the size of the prey. They attack crickets from the side with a narrow distribution of attack angles around 90° relative to the cricket’s body axis. Although most attacks are directed straight ahead, some shrews show a lateralization in their hunting: they preferentially attack with their head turned to their right rather than to their left side.

Temporal attack characteristics. Prey capture occurs very fast, in 80-200 ms per attack, with short inter-attack intervals of around 200 ms (Figure 3 C, D).

Attack dynamics. While first attacks are distributed relatively broadly over the cricket’s body, subsequent attacks are directed more and more precisely to the thorax with the help of corrective head turns that the shrews perform. Thus, shrews can use contact information from a distant body part of the cricket to guide attacks towards their preferred location.

Whisker dependence and tactile shape recognition

Both macro- and microvibrissae are required for hunting, with the macrovibrissae being particularly relevant for attack targeting. Shrews attack a plastic replica of a cricket but not other objects of similar size. Altering the shape of crickets by gluing on additional body parts from donor animals reveals that the jumping legs but not the head are key features in prey recognition. Addition of such “ectopic” jumping legs is highly confusing for shrews and leads to dramatic changes in attack pattern. The anterior thorax, a preferred target in normal crickets, is not attacked at all. However, the number of attacks targeted to the legs greatly increases. In summary, these experiments show that tactile shape cues are both necessary and sufficient for evoking attacks.

Shrews distinguish and memorize prey features and their prey representation is motion and size invariant. Shrew behavior appears to be based on the “Gestaltwahrnehmung” of crickets: they form a global construct of a cricket rather than only recognizing local elements. Thus, tactile object recognition in Etruscan shrews shares characteristics of human visual object recognition, but it proceeds faster and occurs in a 20,000-times-smaller brain.

Experience shapes tactile behavior in shrews

Little is known about the development of behavioral capacities in the somatosensory system. Three lines of evidence suggest that shrew tactile behaviors are not hard-wired but modified by tactile experience. The hunting behavior of young animals differs in subtle but significant ways from the hunting behavior of adults, whisker deprivation in young shrews disrupt the acquisition of normal hunting skills and shrews can acquire new hunting strategies in response to novel prey (Anjum and Brecht, in prep.)

The Etruscan shrew brain

Figure 4: The Etruscan shrew brain. Left: Coronal section through a shrew brain (Nissl stain), scale bar 500 µm. Right: Two-photon microscopy of shrew cortex layer 2, scale bar 20 µm. Neurons are labeled with the green Calcium indicator dye OregonGreenBapta1-AM, which is also taken up by astrocytes. In addition, astrocytes are labeled with the red dye sulforhodamine and thus appear yellow in this overlay.

Histological analyses as well as physiological techniques have been applied to assess the organization of the Etruscan shrew cortex. Based on Nissl and NeuN stains of coronal brain sections (Figure 4, left), the number of neurons is estimated to be ~1 million per cortical hemisphere, the surface area 11 mm² per hemisphere and the volume 4.5 mm³ (Naumann, 2008). The cortex is between 300µm to 600µm thick.

The small brain size of the Etruscan shrew and its thin cortex makes this animal uniquely accessible for imaging based approaches to study brain function. In particular, using two-photon-microscopy might allow visualizing the structure and function of cortical networks in unprecedented completeness (Figure 4, right; Roth-Alpermann, Houweling and Brecht, unpublished).

Cortical sensory areas have been delineated using multi-unit electrophysiological mapping of sensory responses (Brecht et al., submitted). Large parts of Etruscan shrew cortex (i.e. 60% of the total neocortical surface) respond to sensory stimuli. A small visual and a small auditory area have been identified. The majority of recording sites respond to tactile stimuli and more than half of these sites respond to macrovibrissae stimulation.

These findings demonstrate a remarkable degree of tactile specialization in the Etruscan shrew cortex. In comparison to other mammals studied so far, it is clear that the Etruscan shrew is one of the most extreme tactile specialists studied to date.

References

  • Anjum, F and Brecht, M (in preparation). Tactile experience shapes prey-capture behavior in Etruscan shrews.
  • Anjum, F; Turni, H; Mulder, P G H; van der Burg, J and Brecht, M (2006). Tactile guidance of prey capture in Etruscan shrews. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 103(44): 16544-16549.
  • Archibald, J D; Averianov, A O and Ekdale, E G (2001). Late Cretaceous relatives of rabbits, rodents, and other extant eutherian mammals. Nature 414(6859): 62-65.
  • Aulagnier, S et al (2008). Suncus etruscus. In: IUCN 2009. IUCN Red List of Threatened Species, Version 2009.2. http://www.iucnredlist.org. Downloaded on 10 November 2009.
  • Brecht, M; Anjum, F; Naumann, R and Roth-Alpermann, C (submitted). Cortical organization in the Etruscan shrew (Suncus etruscus).
  • Fons, R; Sender, S; Peters, T and Jürgens, K D (1997). Rates of rewarming, heart and respiratory rates and their significance for oxygen transport during arousal from torpor in the smallest mammal, the Etruscan shrew Suncus etruscus. The Journal of Experimental Biology 200(Pt10): 1451-1458.
  • Grünwald, A (1969). Untersuchungen zur Orientierung der Weisszahnspitzmäuse (Soricidae – Crocidurinae). Zeitschrift für vergleichende Physiologie 65: 91-217.
  • Jürgens, K D (2002). Etruscan shrew muscle: The consequences of being small. The Journal of Experimental Biology 205(Pt15): 2161-2166.
  • Naumann, R K (2008). Neuroanatomy of the Etruscan Shrew. Diploma thesis. Humboldt-University, Berlin, Germany.
  • Wilson, D E and Reeder, D A M (2005). Mammal Species of the World: A Taxonomic and Geographic Reference. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Internal references

External links

Movies of Etruscan shrew prey capture

The authors' website

http://www.biotact.org Research project developing novel tactile sensory technologies inspired by the vibrissal sensory systems of mammals.

http://www.activetouch.org Active touch community website.

Personal tools
Namespaces

Variants
Actions
Navigation
Focal areas
Activity
Tools