Interoception

From Scholarpedia
Frederike Hermi Petzschner et al. (2022), Scholarpedia, 17(5):55569. doi:10.4249/scholarpedia.55569 revision #197497 [link to/cite this article]
Jump to: navigation, search

 

Interoception is the sense of the body's internal physiological variables, their integration and interpretation. It is implicated in homeostasis and allostasis, as well as in emotional and self-related processes.


Contents

Definition

Sherrington introduced the term interoceptive to refer to internal sensing, which he distinguished from external sensing and muscle proprioception (Sherrington, 1907). A more recent definition describes interoception as “the overall process of how the nervous system (central and autonomic) senses, interprets and integrates signals originating from within the body, providing a moment-by-moment mapping of the internal landscape of the body across conscious and nonconscious levels.” (Berntson & Khalsa, 2021, Khalsa et al., 2018). These internal signals reflect physiological parameters, such as bodily temperature or the contents, distention and movement of visceral organs, including blood composition and blood pressure.

There are unresolved issues regarding the boundaries of interoception, regarding for instance the inclusion or not of proprioception. Another issue is whether one should consider a sharp boundary, or rather a gradient, between what is internal or external. Discrepancies between authors reflect whether interoception is operationalized according to transducer location, anatomical pathways to the central nervous system, or functional role of the inputs.


Variety of input signals and pathways

Interoceptive information, resulting from the activity of mechanoreceptors, thermoceptors, or chemoceptors, is conveyed via several different pathways. Neural pathways (via unmyelinated (C) or thin (Að) sensory fibres) predominantly travel to the brain via cranial nerves, notably the Vagus Nerve, and via spinal pathways (Berntson & Khalsa, 2021). Humoral information reflecting the chemical composition of blood can also be detected directly by the brain notably via either specialised circumventricular organs or through transport across the blood brain barrier. In addition, the brain may sample its immediate environment, for example, neurons in the preoptic hypothalamus sense local temperature (Tan & Knight, 2018). Inputs may also arise from sensors outside the typical interoceptive domain, e.g. the transduction of respiration via pressure receptors in the nose (Grosmaitre et al., 2007) or the detection of heartbeats by somatosensory pathways (Khalsa et al., 2009, Bishop, 1983, Azzalini et al., 2019).

As compared to exteroception, detailed mechanisms of transduction and anatomo-functional pathways are less well known, notably because they are more difficult to measure experimentally. Finally, signal transduction in interoception occurs on a wider spectrum of time scales, from milliseconds (heartbeat) to several minutes (osmolarity) up to hours (nutrients).


Functions

Interoception serves a primary role in body regulation. By extension, interoception is implicated in reward valuation, perception, emotional processing and consciousness.

Body Regulation

Interoception informs the brain about bodily states, thereby providing the basis for appropriate action selection to regulate these states (Dworkin, 1993). Regulation of bodily states comes in two forms: reactive control (homeostasis) or prospective control (allostasis) (Sterling, 2014). Reactive control occurs when a physiological variable deviates from a normal set range, e.g. sweating in the heat. Prospective control refers to actions that are taken in anticipation of a homeostatic challenge, e.g. increasing the heart rate before running.

Reward and Motivation

Interoceptive states also underpin the determination of rewards and preferences. For instance, depending on the internal state, foods might be considered to be rewarding or aversive (Small et al., 2001). In addition, the willingness or motivation to exert effort to obtain a reward depends on the internal homeostatic need, e.g. increased drive for salty over sweet foods during a state of low osmolarity (Cone et al., 2016), or the amount of energy available to an organism.

Learning and Memory

Interoceptive signals can also promote learning and memory function. For instance, both neural (via the vagus nerve) and endocrine (e.g., hormones that fluctuate with energy status) signalling from the gut to the brain enhances learning and memory function, whereas interference with these signalling pathways has the opposite effect (Suarez et al., 2019).

Emotions

Interoceptive states are also related to emotions since both subjective emotional feelings and objective behavioural expression of emotions are typically coupled with a change in physiological state (Feldman Barrett, 2018, Damasio, 2004). The directionality of the link between interoception and emotion has long been debated (Cannon, 1927): emotions could drive bodily changes or be a consequence of those bodily changes. In fact, the debate has revealed that there are likely to be two-way, even circular interactions.

Self and Consciousness

The constant (conscious or unconscious) monitoring of interoceptive states by the brain has also been proposed to be a basis for a basic form of self—the organism which needs to be fed, regulated and protected (Damasio, 2010). Consciousness, defined as the ability to have subjective experiences, presupposes the existence of an experiencer, which might be related to the basic interoceptive self (Park & Tallon-Baudry, 2014). There is now some experimental evidence that the neural representation of interoceptive (and proprioceptive) variables contributes to consciousness (Azzalini et al., 2019, Seth & Tsakiris, 2018).

Bodily cycles

In addition, bodily cycles, such as the cardiac or respiratory cycles, affect the perception-cognition-action loop - for instance, a visual or auditory stimulus may not be perceived in the same way at the beginning or at the end of the cardiac cycle (Garfinkel & Critchley, 2016). We have increasing experimental evidence for such interactions across different physiological axes. However, the nature of the link between e.g. the cardiac or respiratory cycle and perception, action and cognition remains to be fully elucidated.


Neural Basis

Interoceptive information from the body is conveyed neurally via sensory afferents that travel to the brain via cranial nerves (notably vagus (X) and glossopharyngeal (IX) nerves) and via spinothalamic tracts (Craig, 2009). Interoceptive information primarily targets the brainstem Nucleus of the Solitary Tract (NTS), or directly the thalamus.

In addition, humoral information can be conveyed through transport across the blood brain barrier or detected directly by the brain’s circumventricular organs that carry chemoreceptors sensitive to osmotic, metabolic, and inflammatory status with reciprocal connections to the NTS and hypothalamus (Siso, 2010).

Interoceptive information from the NTS projects through (mainly ventroposterior) medial thalamus to cortex, branching to synapse within pons and midbrain (e.g. parabrachial nucleus, locus coeruleus, hypothalamus) to influence neuromodulatory pathways and subcortical substrates of motivational behaviour (Berntson & Khalsa, 2021).

While there is a fairly good representation of these connections up until the brainstem there are several knowledge gaps with respect to the neural basis of interoceptive information processing beyond that. Several cortical areas have been associated with the (multi-)sensory representation and control of bodily states, including but not limited to, the insula, the somato-sensory cortex, the anterior cingulate cortex and subcortical areas, including the hypothalamus, hippocampus, amygdala and the basal ganglia (Dum et al., 2009). Tracing studies between the periphery and cortex are beginning to bridge this gap (Han et al., 2018).


Measuring interoception

How interoception is measured depends on the interoceptive domains (e.g, cardiac, thermal, gastric). Still, there are a number of challenges common to all domains. First, it is more difficult to manipulate interoceptive signals (e.g. timing and strength of heartbeats) than exteroceptive stimuli (e.g. light intensity) because interoceptive signals are endogenously produced, and cannot always be easily titrated. Second, any manipulation (e.g. exercise) is likely to have multiple consequences at different levels of the system (afferent, integrative, efferent). Third, interoceptive processing is often delayed, with responses occurring within milliseconds up to several hours depending on the system or intervention (e.g. food intake).

Interoceptive measures can be roughly divided into objective vs. subjective as well as direct vs. indirect measures. Subjective measures typically entail asking a (most often human) subject to rate how he or she perceives an interoceptive stimulus or state (e.g., heartbeat tracking, feeling of thirst), or questionnaires about everyday life interoceptive feelings. Objective measures are peripheral (e.g. sweat produced in response to a thermal stimulation), neural (e.g. amplitude of the heartbeat evoked response) or behavioral (e.g. the number of licks in liquid-deprived animals). While all the above are examples of direct measures, there are also indirect measures, such as response time differences as a function of the cardiac cycle phase or changes in choice preferences for more salty liquids during states of low osmolarity.

Disorders related to interoception

Disorders related to interoception can arise from misperception or misrepresentation of interoceptive states. Such misrepresentation can in turn lead to altered motivational or emotional states, or to downstream changes in the regulation of the body which can result in states of chronic dyshomeostasis (Khalsa et al., 2018, Petzschner et al., 2017).

Anxiety and Panic Disorders

Humans with anxiety and panic disorders express threat-related bodily arousal responses and are associated with exaggerated interoceptive attention and often a misinterpretation of the meaning of interoceptive sensations, e.g. catastrophizing about heart rate acceleration.

Eating Disorders

Food intake is guided by the perception and discrimination of interoceptive sensations of hunger and satiety, by unconscious hormonal communication, and by subliminal reinforcing signals that are generated in the periphery (gastrointestinal tract and hepatoportal system) during nutrient digestion to directly regulate central dopamine circuits. Consequently, perturbation of these processes can underlie or maintain eating disorders, including obesity and anorexia nervosa.

Other

In addition, interoceptive dysfunction is proposed to contribute to the etiology and maintenance of stress-sensitive medical disorders (e.g. irritable bowel syndrome, chronic pain and fatigue syndromes), functional neurological conditions (e.g. non-epileptic attacks or functional movement disorder), mood disorders (e.g. depression) and psychiatric symptoms (e.g. disturbances of conscious experience).


Open questions

Despite taking its roots in Sherrington’s work in 1907, the scientific study of interoception is relatively young and has burgeoned in the last 10-20 years, yielding more questions than definite answers. Here, we list a few:

  • What are the optimal approaches for quantifying perceptual processing of interoceptive information across functional axes (e.g. cardiac, respiratory, gastric)?
  • Is interoception a unified system (as the visual, or the auditory system) or should it be decomposed by organs or functions?
  • Is there a (cortical) representation of an integrated interoceptive state, or distributed coding?
  • What are the mechanisms underlying top-down bodily regulation?
  • What is the role of non-neuronal structures and cell types in interoception?
  • What is the developmental trajectory of interoception? Is there maternal interoception of the foetus?
  • What is the link between interoception and emotion?
  • What is the link between interoception and the self?
  • How does interoception influence exteroception, and conversely?
  • What are the boundaries of interoception - are they determined anatomically, functionally?
  • What is the link between interoception and chronic diseases, like cancer?
  • How does the brain sense the amount of energy that is available for effort?
  • How to measure interoception from behavior?


Acknowledgements

This article was supported by the Interoception Research Network, an NIH/NCI-organized workgroup on interoception.


References

Azzalini, Damiano; Rebollo, Ignacio and Tallon-Baudry, Catherine. Visceral Signals Shape Brain Dynamics and Cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 23 (6): 488–509. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2019.03.007. ISSN 13646613. 

Feldman Barrett, Lisa (2018). How emotions are made: the secret life of the brain (First Mariner Book edition ed.). Boston New York: Mariner Books. ISBN 978-1-328-91543-6. 

Berntson, Gary G. and Khalsa, Sahib S.. Neural Circuits of Interoception. Trends in Neurosciences 44 (1): 17–28. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2020.09.011. 

Bishop, V.S. (1983). "Cardiac mechanoreceptors". Handbook of Physiology. pp. 479–555. 

Cannon, W. B. (1927). The James-Lange theory of emotions: a critical examination and an alternative theory. The American Journal of Psychology 39: 106–124. doi:10.2307/1415404. ISSN 1939-8298(Electronic),0002-9556(Print). 

Cone, Jackson J.; Fortin, Samantha M.; McHenry, Jenna A.; Stuber, Garret D.; McCutcheon, James E. and Roitman, Mitchell F. (2016). Physiological state gates acquisition and expression of mesolimbic reward prediction signals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 113 (7): 1943–1948. doi:10.1073/pnas.1519643113. ISSN 1091-6490. PMID: 26831116. 

Craig, B. (2009). How do you feel — now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 10 (1): 59–70. doi:10.1038/nrn2555. ISSN 1471-0048 1471-003X, 1471-0048. 

Damasio, Antonio R. (2004). Descartes' error: emotion, reason and the human brain (18. Druck ed.). New York: Quill. ISBN 978-0-380-72647-9 978-0-399-13894-2. 

Damasio, Antonio R. (2010). Self comes to mind: constructing the conscious brain (1st ed ed.). New York: Pantheon Books. ISBN 978-0-307-37875-0. 

Dum, R. P.; Levinthal, D. J. and Strick, P. L. (2009). The Spinothalamic System Targets Motor and Sensory Areas in the Cerebral Cortex of Monkeys. Journal of Neuroscience 29 (45): 14223–14235. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3398-09.2009. ISSN 1529-2401 0270-6474, 1529-2401. 

Dworkin, B.R. (1993). Learning and Physiological Regulation. 

Garfinkel, Sarah N. and Critchley, Hugo D. (2016). Threat and the Body: How the Heart Supports Fear Processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 20 (1): 34–46. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.005. ISSN 13646613. 

Grosmaitre, Xavier; Santarelli, Lindsey C; Tan, Jie; Luo, Minmin and Ma, Minghong. Dual functions of mammalian olfactory sensory neurons as odor detectors and mechanical sensors. Nature Neuroscience 10 (3): 348–354. doi:10.1038/nn1856. ISSN 1546-1726 1097-6256, 1546-1726. 

Han, Wenfei; Tellez, Luis A.; Perkins, Matthew H.; Perez, Isaac O.; Qu, Taoran; Ferreira, Jozelia; Ferreira, Tatiana L.; Quinn, Daniele; et al. (2018). A Neural Circuit for Gut-Induced Reward. Cell 175 (3): 665–678.e23. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2018.08.049. ISSN 00928674. 

Khalsa, Sahib S.; Adolphs, Ralph; Cameron, Oliver G.; Critchley, Hugo D.; Davenport, Paul W.; Feinstein, Justin S.; Feusner, Jamie D.; Garfinkel, Sarah N.; et al.. Interoception and Mental Health: A Roadmap. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging 3 (6): 501–513. doi:10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.12.004. ISSN 24519022. 

Khalsa, Sahib S; Rudrauf, David; Feinstein, Justin S and Tranel, Daniel. The pathways of interoceptive awareness. Nature Neuroscience 12 (12): 1494–1496. doi:10.1038/nn.2411. ISSN 1546-1726 1097-6256, 1546-1726. 

Park, Hyeong-Dong and Tallon-Baudry, Catherine (2014). The neural subjective frame: from bodily signals to perceptual consciousness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 369 (1641): 20130208. doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0208. ISSN 1471-2970 0962-8436, 1471-2970. 

Petzschner, Frederike H.; Weber, Lilian A.E.; Gard, Tim and Stephan, Klaas E. (2017). Computational Psychosomatics and Computational Psychiatry: Toward a Joint Framework for Differential Diagnosis. Biological Psychiatry 82 (6): 421–430. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.05.012. ISSN 00063223. 

Tan, Chan Lek and Knight, Zachary A. Regulation of Body Temperature by the Nervous System. Neuron 98 (1): 31–48. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2018.02.022. ISSN 08966273. 

Seth, Anil K. and Tsakiris, Manos (2018). Being a Beast Machine: The Somatic Basis of Selfhood. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 22 (11): 969–981. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2018.08.008. ISSN 13646613. 

Sherrington, C.S. (1907). On the proprio-ceptive system, especially in its reflex aspect. 29. pp. 467–482. doi:https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/29.4.467. 

Sisó, Sílvia; Jeffrey, Martin and González, Lorenzo (2010-12). Sensory circumventricular organs in health and disease. Acta Neuropathologica 120 (6): 689–705. doi:10.1007/s00401-010-0743-5. ISSN 1432-0533 0001-6322, 1432-0533. 

Small, D. M.; Zatorre, R. J.; Dagher, A.; Evans, A. C. and Jones-Gotman, M. (2001). Changes in brain activity related to eating chocolate: from pleasure to aversion. Brain: A Journal of Neurology 124 (Pt 9): 1720–1733. doi:10.1093/brain/124.9.1720. ISSN 0006-8950. PMID: 11522575. 

Sterling, Peter. Homeostasis vs Allostasis: Implications for Brain Function and Mental Disorders. JAMA Psychiatry 71 (10): 1192. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.1043. ISSN 2168-622X. 

Suarez, Andrea N.; Noble, Emily E. and Kanoski, Scott E. (2019). Regulation of Memory Function by Feeding-Relevant Biological Systems: Following the Breadcrumbs to the Hippocampus. Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience 12: 101. doi:10.3389/fnmol.2019.00101. ISSN 1662-5099. PMID: 31057368. 


Recommended Reading

Wikipedia Interoception

Book: The interoceptive mind

Handbook of Psychophysiology

Handbook of Physiology

Book: How emotions are made


See also

Scholarpedia Consciousness

Scholarpedia Models of Consciousness

Scholarpedia Theories of Perception

Scholarpedia Autonomic Nervous System

Personal tools
Namespaces

Variants
Actions
Navigation
Focal areas
Activity
Tools